APPENDIX II: TWO CORE PROCESSES

Note: text in red and bold reflect my emphasis.

"For the first time, we now have guidance-based on evidence from sound scientific research-on how best to teach children to read"

Duane Alexander, M.D., Director of the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development
              
(
http://156.40.88.3/new/releases/nrp.htm)

Table of Contents

Phonemic Awareness
Processing Sound/Letter Combinations
The Importance of Phonemic Awareness & Processing Sound/Letter Combinations
The Importance of (the above) to Early Success in Reading

Phonemic Awareness:

From the: National Adult Literacy and Learning Disabilities Center – Phonemic Awareness: Sound Advice for Teachers by Jane Fell Green, Ed.D.      http://www.niwl.org/nalldc/ALLDphonological

It’s the hottest topic in education.  So many studies have been published in the most highly respected education journals that there’s hardly an educator alive who hasn’t heard the term phonological awareness or its related term, phonemic awareness.  But what is this thing? 

From the: National Reading Panel Report (http://www.nrrf.org/rdg_panel_rpt.htm)

Phonemic awareness is knowledge that spoken words are made up of tiny segments of sound, referred to as phonemes. For example, the words "go" and "she" each consist of two phonemes. Phonemic awareness is often confused with phonics, which refers to the process of linking these sounds to the symbols that stand for them, the letters of the alphabet.

From the: Committee on the Prevention of Reading Difficulties in Young Children, National Research Council (http://www.nap.edu/readingroom/books/prdyc/ch2.html)

Spoken words can be phonologically subdivided at several different levels of analysis. These include the syllable (in the word protect, /pro/ and /tEkt/); the onset and rime within the syllable (/pr/ and /o/, and /t/ and /Ekt/, respectively); and the individual phonemes themselves (/p/, /r/, /o/, /t/, /E/, /k/, and /t/). The term phonological awareness refers to a general appreciation of the sounds of speech as distinct from their meaning. When that insight includes an understanding that words can be divided into a sequence of phonemes, this finer-grained sensitivity is termed phonemic awareness.

Children with phonemic awareness are able to discern that camp and soap end with the same sound, that blood and brown begin with the same sound, or, more advanced still, that removing the /m/ from smell leaves sell.

It is also important to clarify the difference between phonological awareness and phonics. Phonics is the term that has long been used among educators to refer to instruction in how the sounds of speech are represented by letters and spellings, for instance, that the letter M represents the phoneme /m/ and the various conventions by which the long sounds of vowels are signaled. Phonics, in short, presumes a working awareness of the phonemic composition of words. In conventional phonics programs, however, such awareness was generally taken for granted, and therein lies the force of the research on phonemic awareness. To the extent that children lack such phonemic awareness, they are unable to internalize usefully their phonics lessons. The resulting symptoms include difficulties in sounding and blending new words, in retaining words from one encounter to the next, and in learning to spell.

From: "Onsets and Rimes": Their Relation to Effective Phonics Information by Dr. Patrick Groff Professor of Education Emeritus San Diego State University NRRF Board Member & Senior Advisor (http://www.nrrf.org/45_onsets_and_rimes.html)

Experimental research on children's conscious awareness of speech sounds in spoken words has been carried out for at least twenty years. This manifest sensitivity to speech sounds is called phonemic (or phonological) awareness (PA). It now is well-established that children who have developed PA gain written word recognition skills better than do children who lack PA. Moreover, it is found that PA is best taught in a direct and systematic fashion. We now know that school beginners' lack of PA is far more detrimental to their progress in learning to read than is their lack of experience with print, as preschoolers.

It thus is apparent that PA is a prerequisite to phonics instruction.

Processing Sound/Letter Combinations:

From the: Committee on the Prevention of Reading Difficulties in Young Children, National Research Council
(http://www.nap.edu/readingroom/books/prdyc/ch2.html)

Visual processes initiate word identification and immediately trigger other processes that complete it, including, most importantly, phonological decoding processes, which concern the correspondences between printed letters and the sounds of the language, especially phonemes, the small sound units within spoken and heard words. The research on reading in alphabetic writing systems has developed an important consensus that phonological decoding is a routine part of skilled word identification.

Word identification research has provided information about how words are understood as well as how their phonological form is initially identified from print.

Addressing the early stages of learning to read, researchers argue that children move from a prereading stage, marked by "reading" environmental print (logos, for example, such as MacDonald's or Pepsi), into true reading through an intermediate stage, referred to as phonetic cue reading (Ehri, 1980, 1991; Ehri and Wilce, 1985, 1987). In this intermediate stage, the child begins to use the phonetic values of the names of letters as a representation of the word. For example, children can learn to read the word "jail" by picking out the salient first and last letters, j and l, and associating the letter names, "jay" and "ell" with sounds heard when the word "jail" is pronounced. This kind of reading is viewed as a primitive form of decoding (or what Gough and Hillinger, 1980, called "deciphering")--decoding because it uses systematic relationships between letters and speech segments in words, and primitive because it is a strategy that ignores some of the letters and also because it maps letter names rather than the phoneme values of the letters. In the full decoding or deciphering stage, children begin to attend to all letters and to map them to phonemes. Although these phonemes are not always the right ones, the child is then in the stage of full productive reading, because he or she is applying the alphabetic principle very generally across encounters with words.

The Importance of Phonemic Awareness and Processing Sound/Letter Combinations:

From the: National Institute of Child Health and Human Development – 1997 Synthesis of Research on Reading (http://www.nrrf.org/synthesis_research.htm)

Converging evidence from all research centers show that deficits in phonemic awareness reflect the core deficit in reading disabilities.

Lack of phonemic awareness seems to be a major obstacle for learning to read. This is true for any language, even Chinese. About 2 in 5 children have some level of difficulty with phonemic awareness. For about 1 in 5 children phonemic awareness does not develop or improve over time. These children never catch up but fall further and further behind in reading and in all academic subjects.

From the: National Adult Literacy and Learning Disabilities Center – Phonemic Awareness: Sound Advice for Teachers by Jane Fell Green, Ed.D. http://www.niwl.org/nalldc/ALLDphonological.html

Phonemic awareness is the most potent predictor of success in learning to read--even more powerfully predictive than I.Q.  Conversely, lack of phonemic awareness is the most powerful determinant of failure to learn to read. 

Moreover, if the letter-sound code (phonics) is not taught, all reliable studies concur that poor readers and nonreaders will not become fluent readers.  The most significant predictor of comprehension is the ability to decode isolated words rapidly, accurately, and fluently.

From the: Committee on the Prevention of Reading Difficulties in Young Children, National Research Council (http://www.nap.edu/readingroom/books/prdyc/ch2.html)

The theoretical and practical importance of phonological awareness for the beginning reader relies not only on logic but also on the results of several decades of empirical research. Early studies showed a strong association between a child's ability to read and the ability to segment words into phonemes (Liberman et al., 1974). Dozens of subsequent studies have confirmed that there is a close relationship between phonemic awareness and reading ability, not just in the early grades (e.g., Ehri and Wilce, 1980, 1985; Perfetti et al., 1987) but throughout the school years (Calfee et al., 1973; Shankweiler et al., 1995). Furthermore, as we discuss in Chapter 4, even prior to formal reading instruction, the performance of kindergartners on tests of phonological awareness is a strong predictor of their future reading achievement (Juel, 1991; Scarborough, 1989; Stanovich, 1986; Wagner et al., 1994).

… research repeatedly demonstrates that, when steps are taken to ensure an adequate awareness of phonemes, the reading and spelling growth of the group as a whole is accelerated and the incidence of reading failure is diminished.

Moreover, as we discuss in later chapters of this report, numerous studies have shown that learning to read can be facilitated by providing explicit instruction that directs children's attention to the phonological structure of words, indicating that phonological awareness plays a causal role in learning to read (see Chapter 6). On the other hand, instruction in alphabetic literacy, particularly regarding the correspondences between letters and phonemes, in turn appears to facilitate further growth in phonological (especially phonemic) awareness.

Theorists such as Share (1995) have argued that becoming skilled in phonological decoding provides the child with a self-teaching mechanism that, along with oral vocabulary knowledge and context, is useful for learning to read words that they have not previously encountered. After a few such correct decodings, these words can be recognized quite automatically. In thinking about the process of learning to read and about how best to frame early reading instruction, it is important to bear in mind these powerful reciprocal influences of reading skill and phonological awareness on each other.

These early connections between print and speech forms can drive a rapid transition to real reading. Indeed, the combination of these print-sound connections along with phonological sensitivity are critical factors in reading acquisition (Bradley and Bryant, 1983; Ehri and Sweet, 1991; Juel et al., 1986; Share, 1995; Tunmer et al., 1988). Studies by Stuart and Coltheart (1988) and Stuart (1990) illustrate the importance of these early phonologically based approaches to reading. Stuart (1990) added to these results by finding that the level of a child's phonological sensitivity corresponded in some detail to the level of achievement in word reading.

CONCLUSION : The achievement of real reading requires knowledge of the phonological structures of language and how the written units connect with the spoken units. Phonological sensitivity at the subword level is important in this achievement. Very early, children who turn out to be successful in learning to read use phonological connection to letters, including letter names, to establish context-dependent phonological connections, which allow productive reading. An important mechanism for this is phonological recoding, which helps the child acquire high-quality word representations. Gains in fluency (automaticity) come with increased experience, as does increased lexical knowledge that supports word identification.

From the: National Adult Literacy and Learning Disabilities Center – Director’s Statement about Phonological Awareness (http://www.niwl.org/nalldc/ALLDphonological)

The most powerful interventions that have been identified for reading disabilities to date consist of a combination of explicit instruction in phonemic awareness, explicit instruction in sound-symbol relationships or in mapping sound to print (the alphabetic principle), and direct and integrated instruction in text reading and comprehension.

From: Education News.org: The relationship between phonics and phonemic awareness Dr. Kerry Hempenstall (http://www.educationnews.org/relationship_between_phonics_and.htm)

In addition to correlational evidence, there have been a number of longitudinal training studies showing that the relationship between phonemic awareness and reading progress is causal. The most famous of these studies, presented in Bradley and Bryant’s seminal paper in 1983, was described by Coltheart (1983) as the "first clear evidence of the mental procedures important in the early stages of learning to read" (p. 421). Subsequent intervention studies (Ball & Blachman, 1988, 1991; Byrne & Fielding-Barnsley, 1989, 1990, 1991, 1993, 1995; Cunningham, 1990; Hatcher, Hulme, & Ellis, 1994; Lundberg, Frost, & Petersen, 1988; Tangel & Blackman, 1992) obtained similar results, and those that employed follow-up have noted the endurance of the effects.

In a number of these studies, the teaching of phonemic awareness has occurred in conjunction with letter-sound instruction, a process described by Hatcher et al. (1994) as a "phonological linkage" (p. 42). Children in dual-input programs demonstrate more improvement in reading and spelling than those exposed to a solely oral phonemic awareness program. Presumably the reason for this advantage lies with the manner in which phonemic awareness provides a signpost to beginning readers that there is a logic to the reading process (Liberman, Shankweiler, & Liberman, 1989). Ehri (1998) asserts that it is not until students appreciate how our alphabet is designed to represent speech in phonemic form that most phonemic awareness development occurs.

Share (1995) has argued that without the induction of the alphabetic principle, skilled reading (implying the use of a generative strategy capable of decoding novel words) will not occur. His view is supported by the finding that dyslexic adult readers (even those with strong orthographic capacities) still demonstrate phonemic awareness deficits, and struggle to decode novel words (Bruck, 1992; Hulme & Snowling, 1992; Pratt & Brady, 1988; Siegel, 1993; Solman & Stanovich, 1992).

Share and Stanovich (1995) assert that mature orthographic strategies are developed through multiple examples of success in decoding phonologically. If one accepts this view, then whole word strategies should not be emphasised, and the instructional focus for older students must still be placed on ensuring letter-sound correspondences, blending and segmenting, and practice. It is further argued that only through such laborious serial letter-by-letter decoding can precise letter-order become entrenched in the orthographic representation that forms the basis for accurate spelling (Adams, 1990; Ehri, 1998; Jorm & Share, 1983; Williams, 1991). Thus, phonics instruction continues to play an important role for older children and adults.

National Center to Improve the Tools of Educators Emergent Literacy: Synthesis of the Research (
http://idea.uoregon.edu/~ncite/documents/techrep/tech19.html)

Letter knowledge, which provides the basis for forming connections between the letters in spellings and the sounds in pronunciations, has been identified as a strong predictor of reading success (Ehri & Sweet, 1991) and has traditionally been a very important component of reading readiness programs (van Kleeck, 1990). Knowing the alphabet and its related sounds is associated with beginning literacy. In fact, letter knowledge measured at the beginning of kindergarten was one of two best predictors of reading achievement at the end of kindergarten and first grade - the other predictor was phonemic segmentation skill (Share, Jorm, Maclean, and Matthews, cited in Ehri & Sweet 1991).

From the: U.S. House of Representatives - Committee on Education and the Workforce September 26, 2000:

Testimony of Donald N. Langenberg, Chairman of the National Reading Panel (NRP)
(http://edworkforce.house.gov/hearings/106th/fc/literacy92600/langenberg.htm)

To become good readers, children must develop phonemic awareness, phonics skills, the ability to read words in text in an accurate and fluent manner, and the ability to apply comprehension strategies consciously and deliberately as they read.

Testimony of Ms. Linda Butler, Professional Development Specialist • NICHD Early Interventions Project, Washington, DC (http://edworkforce.house.gov/hearings/106th/fc/literacy92600/butler.htm)

Many teachers have seen immediate and dramatic improvement in their classes when they apply research-based methods. A very important focus of coursework has been in-depth study of phoneme awareness, phonics, and the structure of the English language.

Testimony of Mrs. Pam Barret First Grade Teacher and NRRF 1998 Teacher of the Year (http://edworkforce.house.gov/hearings/106th/fc/literacy92600/barret.htm)

In order to read, the student must learn phonics, which is the sound to symbol association and the skill of blending these sounds together to make a word. In addition, they need to understand phonemic awareness, which is the ability to hear sounds within a spoken word, and to understand that the sounds correspond to letters. This is a vital precursor to phonics.

The Importance of Phonemic Awareness & Phonological Processing To Early Reading Success:

From: What Reading Does For The Mind: By Anne E. Cunningham and Keith E. Stanovich – Published in the American Educator/American Federation of Teachers Spring/Summer 1998 (http://www.aft.org/publications/american_educator/spring_sum98/cunningham.pdf)

The term “Matthew effects” is taken from the Biblical passage that describes a rich-get-richer and poor-get-poorer phenomenon. Applying this concept to reading, we see that very early in the reading process poor readers, who experience greater difficulty in breaking the spelling-to-sound code, begin to be exposed to much less text than their more skilled peers.

From the standpoint of a reciprocal model of reading development, this means that many cognitive differences observed between readers of differing skill may in fact be consequences of differential practice that itself resulted from early differences in the speed of initial reading acquisition. The increased reading experiences of children who master the spelling-to-sound code early thus might have important positive feedback effects that are denied the slowly progressing reader.

 

©Copyright 2001- 2003: Training Wheels for Literacy & Implicity